Hey
everyone!
Have you ever felt that you aren't completely satisfied with your personality? Or maybe you're satisfied with it, but someone close to you has suggested you shouldn't be? Or maybe you're just curious what it'd be like to be different for a while.
Today's blog highlights my experience with this.
I've
always been a reserved man. The "strong, silent type." A stoic person
who never lets his true feelings show through. Always taking the rational
approach, only making decisions that maximize an automatic, unconscious
benefit/cost analysis.
Or
at least, that's what introspection tells me I am. This is the process
of thinking about ourselves and our actions, and drawing conclusions from them.
Except in reality, they aren't conclusions. They're guesses. According to Nisbett and
Wilson (1977), we rarely know the real reasons for our actions. He let people
sample four pairs of panty hose on a table and pick the one they thought was
best. People tended to pick the one on the far right. They would say, "Oh
this one's softer," or "It has a better texture." Except there
was a catch. The panty hose were all the same. So when people thought that one
pair had better qualities, they were wrong. It turned out people just like
things that are located to the right.
So
maybe my guesses about myself aren't right. Maybe I'm not so rational in my
thinking after all. Maybe it leads me astray. Besides, my girlfriend has told
me for years that I should be less inhibited and not think about everything.
That's
what I decided to change.
I
tried to keep from keeping such control over myself. I attempted to just
"go" for things that didn't matter. Carpe diem. YOLO. It made me more
outgoing, impulsive, emotional. I also had a great time acting, which put me in
a good mood. (I probably just seemed drunk throughout half of this.)
In
the locker room before weight training, an older man came in while I was
changing. We exchanged hellos, which is where I'd ordinarily leave it. But
emboldened by my newfound carefree nature, I introduced myself and talked to
him for a few minutes before I had to go. Absolutely nothing came of it, but he
seemed grateful for the conversation. While I never had any other interesting
events happen from my outgoing nature, I felt happier in general.
I
attribute this increase in happiness to the fact that I attempted to make eye
contact with and smile at everyone I saw. There's a concept known as the facial
feedback hypothesis, which suggests that smiling actually causes you to be
happier (Laird,
1974) Essentially, if you make yourself smile enough, your brain thinks
to itself, "Why am I smiling? I only smile when I'm happy. Therefore, I
must be happy," and presto, you're happy. It is a fascinating phenomenon.
If I learned one thing from this experience, it would be that this trick works.
And I will probably employ it for the rest of my life.
The
next big behavior change resulting from my intentional disinhibition,
impulsiveness, was also fun. That is probably not a surprise to anyone. I
"aggressively marketed" T-shirts to hapless passersby outside
the commons, played a ridiculous video game about racing Volkswagen beetles
with friends instead of working on homework (I was able to rationalize it as
working on this assignment), and acted like I was going to completely
blow off an essay.
It was good fun. But there's no way I could keep that up.
Acting with less self-discipline seemed like it would be extremely self-destructive in the long term, such as by ignoring homework. Some people seem to be able to operate without any stress despite failing to uphold their responsibilities. I, for better or worse, am not one of them.
Responsibility is one of my schematic traits. That means I think it's incredibly important, and I pay attention to it and judge it in myself and others (Markus, 1977). Forcing myself to act in the opposite manner was draining and stressful. And because it's a schematic trait, I think it would be a terrible decision to attempt to embody making impulsive decisions and living in the moment. I feel bad enough that I let myself procrastinate on homework, even if it was for science.
Another feature of schematic traits is that we filter our memory such that we only see ourselves with the traits we want to have. I can clearly remember the times I have been proactive regarding assignments, but specific examples of procrastination are harder to bring up. I automatically ignored the events that might counter the notion that I'm not responsible.
The same applies here. I loved playing that stupid Beetle game. I spent an hour learning a new drum rudiment instead of writing my last blog post. I went to the store with my roommate to prepare for APO Senior Wills when I had 118 pages of African History to read that night. I formed flour into an imitation brick of cocaine as a gag gift to a hyperactive friend while another friend sat on my couch waiting to work on chemistry together. But I'm not irresponsible, no; I was just pretending to be for the sake of an assignment. That rationalization let me take it further than I intended, causing me to miss quite a few hours of sleep fixing those mistakes as my responsible old self came back from his little vacation and found the house a wreck.
A couple of my friends noticed, and even made a few sarcastic comments about it. "Shouldn't you be reading instead of going to the store?" "Why did you make me play Beetle Adventure?" (I didn't make him.) This was probably the most out-of-character behavior for me. It was fun while it lasted.
The
last big change included in my mission of controlling myself less appeared most
clearly when commiserating with a friend about an upcoming assignment in a
different class. I hadn’t fully committed to this idea yet at the time, and I
wasn’t sure if it would work. I had no other ideas. As a result of that plus
the other assignment, I was feeling a little bit of stress. That’s when I
decided to just go with it. It would be like a pilot test of my idea.
I
probably played it up a bit, in reality. I got unreasonably frustrated with the
assignment and ranted to my friend for around twenty minutes about how unreasonable
it was and how little it amounted to and how I should just not do it.
Eventually, he got frustrated and quit trying to talk me down, because I was
clearly having none of it. I was a little tempted by my own rationalization,
which was especially interesting to me.
I
believe one of the reasons I was able to make these changes for a time is that
I am a high self-monitor. Self-monitoring
is the ability to change your behavior in order to adapt to whatever social
situation you’re in (Snyder, 1974). For example, people who score high on tests
of self-monitoring tend to be good actors. That’s exactly what this project
required.
The
effects of that acting, such as being happier, showed me just how easily you
can change how you see yourself if you put effort into it. Even though I
approached this assignment with a bit of skepticism regarding how it would
benefit me, I had fun experimenting and am now glad I experienced it.
Although
some of the changes didn’t suit me, such as the impulsivity, I liked how it
felt to be more outgoing. I will continue to smile at random passersby, and I
will continue to trick my brain into making me a happier person.
————————————————————
Laird,
J. D. (1974). Self-attribution of emotion: The effects of expressive behavior
on the quality of emotional experience. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 29, 475–486.
Markus,
H. (1977). Self-schemata and processing information about the self. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 35, 63–78.
Nisbett,
R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal
reports on mental processes. Psychological
Review, 84, 231–259.
Snyder,
M. (1974). The self-monitoring of expressive behavior. Journal
of
Personality and Social Psychology, 30, 526–537.
(N
= 1300 words)
No comments:
Post a Comment